簡易檢索 / 詳目顯示

研究生: 洪晨瑜
Hung, Chen-Yu
論文名稱: 個人日常生活多工自我效能、偏好、頻率與日常生活多工素養之關聯性研究
The relationship between everyday multitasking self-efficacy, preference, behavior and multitasking literacies
指導教授: 劉子鍵
Liu, Tzu-Chien
學位類別: 碩士
Master
系所名稱: 教育心理與輔導學系
Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling
論文出版年: 2019
畢業學年度: 107
語文別: 中文
論文頁數: 60
中文關鍵詞: 日常生活多工自我效能偏好媒體多工計畫能力前瞻性記憶回溯性記憶
英文關鍵詞: everyday multitasking, media multitasking, preference, plan, prospective memory, retrospective memory, self-efficacy
DOI URL: http://doi.org/10.6345/NTNU201900026
論文種類: 學術論文
相關次數: 點閱:113下載:5
分享至:
查詢本校圖書館目錄 查詢臺灣博碩士論文知識加值系統 勘誤回報
  • 在這個科技快速成長的世代,日常生活的多工儼然已經成為我們生活的新的行為模式。為了能夠在這樣一個資訊爆炸、干擾頻繁與訊息快速流通的世代下更有效率的生存,我們常常需要以多工的方式來解決更多的問題,藉此更有效的處理這些即時的訊息。故本研究旨在探究個體的日常生活多工的習慣與其日常生活多工素養之關係。我們認為在日常生活多工情境下,個體對於平時從事日常生活多工行為的自我效能、偏好與頻率可能與其在日常生活多工素養有關聯性。
    本研究之研究參與對象為台北市大專院校之學生41名。使用的測量工具有「日常生活多工自我效能量表」、「日常生活多工偏好量表」、「日常生活多工頻率量表」和「日常生活多工素養評量平台」;其中,「日常生活多工素養評量平台」之評量內容包含個體的計畫能力、前瞻性記憶、回溯性記憶與日常生活多工實作表現。
    主要研究結果如下:
    一、 在日常生活多工素養中,其計畫能力、前瞻性記憶、回溯性記憶與其日常生活多工實作表現有顯著正相關。
    二、 在日常生活多工素養中,其計畫能力、前瞻性記憶與回溯性記憶彼此間有顯著正相關。
    三、 日常生活多工自我效能、偏好、頻率與計畫能力、前瞻性記憶、回溯性記憶與日常生活多工素養並無顯著相關。
    四、 低日常生活多工自我效能組別之自我效能與前瞻性記憶、回溯性記憶和日常生活多工實作表現有顯著負相關。
    五、 低日常生活多工偏好組別之偏好與計畫能力有顯著負相關。

    Everyday multitasking occurs anytime and anywhere in this modern age of advanced technology. In order to manage the speedy exchange of information more efficiently, multitasking behavior has without doubt become a way of living. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to understand the relationship between participants’ multitasking personal habit including everyday multitasking self-efficiency, preference and behavior pattern and their multitasking literacies including the abilities of plan, prospective, retrospective and performance.
    Participants for this study included 41 undergraduate students in Taipei. The variables of multitasking self-efficiency, preference and behavior pattern were measured by “Everyday Multitasking Self-Efficacy Scale, EMSES”, “Everyday Multitasking Preference Scale, EMPS” and “Everyday Multitasking Behavior Pattern Scale, EMBPS”. The variables of multitasking literacies were measured by “Everyday Multitasking Literacy Assessment Platform”.
    This study found that: (1) in everyday multitasking literacies, the abilities of plan, prospective memory and retrospective memory have significant positive correlation with everyday multitasking performance. (2) In everyday multitasking literacies, the ability of plan, prospective memory and retrospective memory have significant positive correlation. (3) Everyday multitasking self-efficacy, preference and behavior pattern have no significant correlation with everyday multitasking literacies. (4) Low everyday multitasking self-efficacy has significant negative correlation with prospective memory, retrospective memory and everyday multitasking performance. (5) Low everyday multitasking preference has significant negative correlation with plan.

    致謝詞 i 中文摘要 ii 英文摘要 iii 目次 v 表次 vii 圖次 viii 第一章 緒論 1 第一節 研究背景與目的動機 1 第二節 研究目的與研究問題 4 第三節 名詞解釋 5 第二章 文獻探討 7 第一節 日常生活多工素養理論與其相關研究 7 第二節 日常生活多工自我效能與日常生活多工素養間的關聯性 12 第三節 日常生活多工偏好與日常生活多工素養之間的關聯性 14 第四節 日常生活多工頻率與日常生活多工表現之間的關聯性 16 第三章 研究方法 19 第一節 研究架構 19 第二節 研究參與者 20 第三節 研究工具 21 第四節 研究程序 24 第五節 資料處理與統計分析 27 第四章 研究結果 28 第一節 大專院校學生日常生活多工之現況分析 28 第二節 日常生活多工自我效能、偏好、頻率與日常生活多工素養之相關 30 第三節 高低日常生活多工自我效能、偏好、頻率與日常生活多工素養之相關分析 33 第五章 討論 38 第六章 結論與建議 42 第一節 研究結論 42 第二節 限制與建議 44 參考文獻 47 中文部分 47 西文部分 47 附錄 57 附錄一 日常生活多工自我效能量表 57 附錄二 日常生活多工偏好量表 58 附錄三 日常生活多工頻率量表 59

    一、中文文獻
    劉子鍵(2018)。日常生活多工素養的評量與教學。科技部專題研究計畫第二年期中成果報告。(編號:MOST 105-2511-S-003-026-MY3)
    劉子鍵(2016)。日常生活多工素養的評量與教學。科技部專題研究計畫(三年期,編號:MOST 105-2511-S-003-026-MY3)。

    二、英文文獻
    Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(8), 971-979.
    Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Knight, C., & Henman, C. (2003). Ecological validity of a simplified version of the multiple errands shopping test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(1), 31-44.
    Alzahabi, R., & Becker, M. W. (2013). The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1485-1495.
    Armstrong, G. B., Boiarsky, G. A., & Mares, M. L. (1991). Background television and reading performance. Communications Monographs, 58(3), 235-253.
    Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American psychologist, 37(2), 122-147.
    Bandura, A. (1986). The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. Journal of social and clinical psychology, 4(3), 359-373.
    Basoglu, K. A., Fuller, M. A., & Sweeney, J. T. (2009). Investigating the effects of computer mediated interruptions: An analysis of task characteristics and interruption frequency on financial performance. International journal of accounting information systems, 10(4), 177-189.
    Bluedorn, A. C., Kalliath, T. J., Strube, M. J., & Martin, G. D. (1999). Polychronicity and the Inventory of Polychronic Values (IPV) The development of an instrument to measure a fundamental dimension of organizational culture. Journal of managerial psychology, 14(3/4), 205-231.
    Bluedorn, A. C., Kaufman, C. F., & Lane, P. M. (1992). How many things do you like to do at once? An introduction to monochronic and polychronic time. The Executive, 6(4), 17-26.
    Bowman, L. L., Levine, L. E., Waite, B. M., & Gendron, M. (2010). Can students really multitask? An experimental study of instant messaging while reading. Computers & Education, 54(4), 927-931.
    Brooks, S. (2015). Does personal social media usage affect efficiency and well-being? Computers in Human Behavior, 46, 26-37.
    Burgess, P. W. (2000). Real-world multitasking from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance, 20(18), 465-472.
    Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The relationship between prospective and retrospective memory: Neuropsychological evidence. Cognitive models of memory, 5, 249-256.
    Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., de Lacy Costello, A., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38(6), 848-863.
    Calderwood, C., Ackerman, P. L., & Conklin, E. M. (2014). What else do college students “do” while studying? An investigation of multitasking. Computers & Education, 75, 19-29.
    Capdeferro, N., Romero, M., & Barberà, E. (2014). Polychronicity: review of the literature and a new configuration for the study of this hidden dimension of online learning. Distance Education, 35(3), 294-310.
    Clayson, D. E., & Haley, D. A. (2013). An introduction to multitasking and texting: Prevalence and impact on grades and GPA in marketing classes. Journal of Marketing Education, 35(1), 26-40.
    Cohen, G., & Conway, M. A. (2007). Memory in the real world: Psychology Press.
    Cohens, G. (1993). Memory and ageing. Advances in psychology, 100, 419-438.
    Conte, J. M., & Gintoft, J. N. (2005). Polychronicity, big five personality dimensions, and sales performance. Human Performance, 18(4), 427-444.
    Conte, J. M., & Jacobs, R. R. (2003). Validity evidence linking polychronicity and big five personality dimensions to absence, lateness, and supervisory performance ratings. Human Performance, 16(2), 107-129.
    Courage, M. L., Bakhtiar, A., Fitzpatrick, C., Kenny, S., & Brandeau, K. (2015). Growing up multitasking: The costs and benefits for cognitive development. Developmental Review, 35, 5-41.
    Dixon, A. L., & Schertzer, S. M. (2005). Bouncing back: How salesperson optimism and self-efficacy influence attributions and behaviors following failure. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 25(4), 361-369.
    Drews, F. A., Yazdani, H., Godfrey, C. N., Cooper, J. M., & Strayer, D. L. (2009). Text messaging during simulated driving. Human factors, 51(5), 762-770.
    Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 717-726.
    Ellis, J., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2000). Prospective memory in 2000: Past, present, and future directions. Applied cognitive psychology, 14(7). 1-9
    Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can't not believe everything you read. Journal of personality and social psychology, 65(2), 221-233.
    Gupta, R., Kar, B. R., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). Development of task switching and post-error-slowing in children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5(1), 38.
    Hecht, T. D., & Allen, N. J. (2005). Exploring links between polychronicity and well-being from the perspective of person–job fit: Does it matter if you prefer to do only one thing at a time? Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 98(2), 155-178.
    Hembrooke, H., & Gay, G. (2003). The laptop and the lecture: The effects of multitasking in learning environments. Journal of computing in higher education, 15(1), 46-64.
    Hodges, S., Williams, L., Berry, E., Izadi, S., Srinivasan, J., Butler, A., ... & Wood, K. (2006). SenseCam: A retrospective memory aid. International Conference on Ubiquitous Computing (pp. 177-193). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.
    Ilgen, D. R., & Pulakos, E. D. (1999). The Changing Nature of Performance: Implications for Staffing, Motivation, and Development. Frontiers of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Inc.
    Jacobsen, W. C., & Forste, R. (2011). The wired generation: Academic and social outcomes of electronic media use among university students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(5), 275-280.
    Junco, R. (2012). In-class multitasking and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(6), 2236-2243.
    Junco, R., & Cotten, S. R. (2012). No A 4 U: The relationship between multitasking and academic performance. Computers & Education, 59(2), 505-514.
    Kaufman, C. F., Lane, P. M., & Lindquist, J. D. (1991). Exploring more than 24 hours a day: A preliminary investigation of polychronic time use. Journal of consumer research, 18(3), 392-401.
    Kaufman-Scarborough, C., & Lindquist, J. D. (1999). Time management and polychronicity: Comparisons, contrasts, and insights for the workplace. Journal of managerial psychology, 14(3/4), 288-312.
    Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2008). Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives. New York: Psychology Press.
    König, C. J., & Waller, M. J. (2010). Time for reflection: A critical examination of polychronicity. Human Performance, 23(2), 173-190.
    Kononova, A., & Chiang, Y.-H. (2015). Why do we multitask with media? Predictors of media multitasking among Internet users in the United States and Taiwan. Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 31-41.
    Kononova, A., Joo, E., & Yuan, S. (2016). If I choose when to switch: Heavy multitaskers remember online content better than light multitaskers when they have the freedom to multitask. Computers in Human Behavior, 65, 567-575.
    Lam, Y. S., Man, D. W., Tam, S. F., & Weiss, P. L. (2006). Virtual reality training for stroke rehabilitation. NeuroRehabilitation, 21(3), 245-253.
    Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1986). Self-efficacy in the prediction of academic performance and perceived career options. Journal of counseling psychology, 33(3), 265-269.
    Lin, L. (2009). Breadth-biased versus focused cognitive control in media multitasking behaviors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(37), 15521-15522.
    Lindbeck, A., & Snower, D. J. (2000). Multitask learning and the reorganization of work: From tayloristic to holistic organization. Journal of labor economics, 18(3), 353-376.
    Logie, R., Law, A., Trawley, S., & Nissan, J. (2010). Multitasking, working memory and remembering intentions. Psychologica Belgica, 50(3-4), 309-326.
    Logie, R. H., Trawley, S., & Law, A. (2011). Multitasking: Multiple, domain-specific cognitive functions in a virtual environment. Memory & cognition, 39(8), 1561-1574.
    Mäntylä, T. (1996). Activating actions and interrupting intentions: Mechanisms of retrieval sensitization in prospective memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. 0. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications, (pp. 93-113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Maylor, E. A. (1993). Aging and forgetting in prospective and retrospective memory tasks. Psychology and Aging, 8(3), 420-428.
    Maylor, E. A., Smith, G., Sala, S. D., & Logie, R. H. (2002). Prospective and retrospective memory in normal aging and dementia: An experimental study. Memory & cognition, 30(6), 871-884.
    McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied cognitive psychology, 14(7), S127-S144.
    Mioni, G., Rendell, P. G., Stablum, F., Gamberini, L., & Bisiacchi, P. S. (2015). Test–retest consistency of Virtual Week: A task to investigate prospective memory. Neuropsychological rehabilitation, 25(3), 419-447.
    Miotto, E. C., & Morris, R. G. (1998). Virtual planning in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Cortex, 34(5), 639-657.
    Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(37), 15583-15587.
    Owen, A. M. (1997). Cognitive planning in humans: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neuropharmacological perspectives. Progress in neurobiology, 53(4), 431-450.
    Pool, M. M., Koolstra, C. M., & Voort, T. H. (2003). The impact of background radio and television on high school students' homework performance. Journal of Communication, 53(1), 74-87.
    Poposki, E. M., & Oswald, F. L. (2010). The multitasking preference inventory: Toward an improved measure of individual differences in polychronicity. Human Performance, 23(3), 247-264.
    Rose, N. S., Luo, L., Bialystok, E., Hering, A., Lau, K., & Craik, F. I. (2015). Cognitive processes in the Breakfast Task: Planning and monitoring. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 69(3), 252-263.
    Rosen, C. (2008). The myth of multitasking. The New Atlantis, 20, 105-110.
    Salvucci, D. D., & Taatgen, N. A. (2008). Threaded cognition: an integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. Psychological review, 115(1), 101-130.
    Sanderson, K. R., Bruk‐Lee, V., Viswesvaran, C., Gutierrez, S., & Kantrowitz, T. (2013). Multitasking: Do preference and ability interact to predict performance at work? Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 86(4), 556-563.
    Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114(2), 727-741.
    Sherer, M., Maddux, J. E., Mercandante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & Rogers, R. W. (1982). The self-efficacy scale: Construction and validation. Psychological reports, 51(2), 663-671.
    Slocombe, T. E., & Bluedorn, A. C. (1999). Organizational behavior implications of the congruence between preferred polychronicity and experienced work-unit polychronicity. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20, 75-99.
    Spink, A., Cole, C., & Waller, M. (2008). Multitasking behavior. Annual review of information science and technology, 42(1), 93-118.
    Staples, D. S., Hulland, J. S., & Higgins, C. A. (1999). A self-efficacy theory explanation for the management of remote workers in virtual organizations. Organization Science, 10(6), 758-776.
    Strayer, D. L., & Drew, F. A. (2004). Profiles in driver distraction: Effects of cell phone conversations on younger and older drivers. Human factors, 46(4), 640-649.
    Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone–induced driver distraction. Current directions in psychological science, 16(3), 128-131.
    Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological science, 12(6), 462-466.
    Sujan, H., Weitz, B. A., & Kumar, N. (1994). Learning orientation, working smart, and effective selling. The Journal of Marketing, 58, 39-52.
    Thompson, L. F., Meriac, J. P., & Cope, J. G. (2002). Motivating online performance: The influences of goal setting and Internet self-efficacy. Social Science Computer Review, 20(2), 149-160.
    Uncapher, M. R., Thieu, M. K., & Wagner, A. D. (2016). Media multitasking and memory: Differences in working memory and long-term memory. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 23(2), 483-490.
    Wallis, C. (2006). The multitasking generation. Time Magazine, 167(13), 48-55.
    Wang, Z., & Tchernev, J. M. (2012). The “myth” of media multitasking: Reciprocal dynamics of media multitasking, personal needs, and gratifications. Journal of Communication, 62(3), 493-513.
    Ward, G., & Morris, R. (2005). Introduction to the psychology of planning. In R. Morris &G. Ward (Eds.), The cognitive psychology of planning (pp. 1-34). Hove: Psychology Press.
    Wasson, C. (2004). Multitasking during virtual meetings. Human Resource Planning, 27(4), 47-61.
    Williams, J. A., Berg, H., Gerber, H., Miller, M., Cox, D., Votteler, N., & McGuire, M. (2011). I get distracted by their being distracted: The etiquette of in-class texting. Eastern Educational Journal, 40(1), 48-56.
    Yap, J. Y., & Lim, S. W. H. (2013). Media multitasking predicts unitary versus splitting visual focal attention. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(7), 889-902.

    下載圖示
    QR CODE